The deepening engagement of sociolinguistics and variation studies with
language change has brought these fields into close contact with the traditional
discipline of historical linguistics. Initially, these were complementary
approaches to diachronic questions. Historical linguistics focused on real-time
data, used written evidence (necessarily, since no sound recordings of speech
existed prior to the late nineteenth century), and dealt with completed changes
across large-scale time spans€ – change across centuries and millennia. The
focus on completed changes typically implied invariant, categorical models
and descriptions. Variationist studies introduced the use of apparent-time evidence,
focused on speech, and dealt with changes in progress, over shorter
time spans€– decades and generations€– using quantitative models and descriptions.
But more recently, this neat division of labour and focus has eroded.
Variationist analyses have been conducted of documentary evidence from
times long past. Quantitative approaches have been brought to bear on completed
long-term changes using written materials to study their time course and
the variation that occurred while they were in progress. Sociolinguistic models
of the mechanisms of change have illuminated historical questions. Whereas
traditional historical linguistics sought, in effect, to use the past to explain the
present, the addition of the variationist perspective to diachronic research has
also, in Labov’s words (1994:€9), made it possible to ‘use the present to explain
the past’.
Particularly noteworthy examples of the fruitfulness of this fusion of variation
and diachrony have occurred in research on historical syntax, such as
the previously mentioned work of Kroch on English, and other studies on languages
as diverse as Yiddish (Santorini 1993), Greek (Ann Taylor 1994), and
Portuguese (Tarallo 1996). Phonological change is less amenable to this kind
of approach, because of the limitations of orthographic evidence; nevertheless,
some fruitful work has been undertaken, such as Toon’s study of ‘the politics
of early Old English sound change’ (1983).
The principal impact of variation research on historical linguistics, however,
may be less methodological and empirical, and more theoretical. The
variationist perspective has had little impact on the comparative method and
the reconstruction of proto-languages, but understanding that variation is an
essential way station in the course of change has substantial implications for
evaluating the plausibility of the changes that are postulated and their social
settings. For example, change-in-progress studies show that the period of variation
can last for a long time, and multiple changes may be underway simultaneously.
This has implications for reconstructing the sequencing of events (e.g.
chain shifts). Contemporary variables sometimes exhibit lexical conditioning
or irregularity, with implications for the regularity of sound change. Studies of
the sociolinguistic types of change have implications for evaluating prior language
contact. The transmission of lexical items, for example, implies borrowing
as a primary mechanism of language change; hence in England after the
Norman conquest, the huge inventory of French loanwords in English implies
that native speakers of English were borrowing from French, while the paucity
of phonological and syntactic effects suggests that Norman accents had very
little impact on the English of descendants of the conquerors who underwent
language shift. By comparison, English in India, which shows phonological
characteristics common in Indian languages, such as retroflex consonants, and
syntactic phenomena such as an invariant tag question ‘isn’t it’, is a case in
which the main mechanism of change was imposition.
A significant consequence of the variationist perspective in historical studies
has been the development of quantified models of change. Yang (2001) is a
noteworthy example of this trend. Treating syntactic change as the product of
the interaction between the distribution of syntactic structures in the input and
the choices that child language learners face in their construction of a mental
grammar, Yang proposes a probabilistic model of grammar competition that
drives change forward along an S-shaped time course. The model crucially
depends on variation:€child language learners do not construct a single, static,
invariant grammar to account for all the facts they encounter; rather, they entertain
multiple alternatives, and select among them probabilistically.
Another significant contribution of variationist studies to historical linguistics
is the refined view they permit of the stages of change. Conventional historical
studies, relying on reconstruction from fragmentary evidence, typically
account only for the endpoints of a change; a diachronic statement like x → y
tells us that an early form x is realised centuries later as y, but gives no perspective
on what happens during the intervening years. But synchronic studies of
changes in progress make it possible to investigate triggering events and onsets
of change (the actuation phase), and subsequent expansion of the innovation
(the implementation phase).
Actuation appears to involve both social and linguistic factors; thus Labov
(2010) attributes the original generalised tensing of /æ/ in the Inland North of
American English to a social event:€ the early nineteenth-century mixture in
north central New York state of speakers coming from several different dialect
regions (including New England and southern New York), during the construction
of the Erie Canal. These source dialects had different contexts for /æ/
tensing. The new communities that emerged from this mixture koinéised these
conflicting patterns by tensing /æ/ in all contexts. The completion of the Erie
Canal provided the pathway to settlement of the Upper Midwest, disseminating
the new vowel phonology across a wide area. The tensed /æ/ vowel subsequently
raised, vacating the low front corner of the vowel space; this provided a
linguistic trigger for the fronting of the other low vowels.
Analysing Variation in English- Edited by Warren Maguire and April McMahon
Cambridge University Press, New York
Chapter 8 Variation and change- Gregory R. Guy
8.4- Variation and change and historical linguistics- Pages 193,194
Nenhum comentário:
Postar um comentário